Abstract
BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: The training to acquire or practicing to perform a skill, which may lead to structural changes in the brain, is called experience-dependent structural plasticity. The main purpose of this cross-sectional study was to investigate the presence of experience-dependent structural plasticity in mathematicians’ brains, which may develop after long-term practice of mathematic thinking.
MATERIALS AND METHODS: Twenty-six volunteer mathematicians, who have been working as academicians, were enrolled in the study. We applied an optimized method of voxel-based morphometry in the mathematicians and the age- and sex-matched control subjects. We assessed the gray and white matter density differences in mathematicians and the control subjects. Moreover, the correlation between the cortical density and the time spent as an academician was investigated.
RESULTS: We found that cortical gray matter density in the left inferior frontal and bilateral inferior parietal lobules of the mathematicians were significantly increased compared with the control subjects. Furthermore, increase in gray matter density in the right inferior parietal lobule of the mathematicians was strongly correlated with the time spent as an academician (r = 0.84; P < .01). Left-inferior frontal and bilateral parietal regions are involved in arithmetic processing. Inferior parietal regions are also involved in high-level mathematic thinking, which requires visuospatial imagery, such as mental creation and manipulation of 3D objects.
CONCLUSION: The voxel-based morphometric analysis of mathematicians’ brains revealed increased gray matter density in the cortical regions related to mathematic thinking. The correlation between cortical density increase and the time spent as an academician suggests experience-dependent structural plasticity in mathematicians’ brains.
Genetic, epigenetic, and environmental factors interact to produce the fine structure of the human brain.1–4 Genetically determined cortical architecture may need to undergo functional and structural changes to meet the increasing demands of a complex environment. The training to acquire or practicing to perform a skill may lead to structural changes in the brain, which is called experience-dependent structural plasticity. A previous MR imaging study revealed that memorizing a huge amount of spatial information increased the cortical gray matter density in the right hippocampus of the human brain, which represents the spatial navigation center.1 Thus, the human brain can make regional structural reorganizations associated with experience and learning.3,5–8
Music is one of the most fascinating products of the human brain. Because becoming a professional musician requires a long, intense, and highly specific training during the early periods of brain maturation, the musician's brain has been used as a model to study the development of experience-dependent structural plasticity.9,10 During their professional training period, musicians learn to convert musical symbols to complex motor actions, memorize long and complex musical artworks, and acquire the capability of perfect tone discrimination. The performances of these very complex tasks cause experience-dependent adaptive changes in the task-related regions of their brain.9,11–16 The morphometric MR studies conducted in the musicians revealed the presence of increased gray matter density in the motor and auditory cortex and the cerebellum compared with nonmusicians.15 It has been proposed that long-term and intense practice by pianists to perform sequential motor tasks with high accuracy resulted in experience-dependent adaptive changes in the related regions of their brains. Results of another voxel-based morphometric study revealed increased gray matter density in the left inferior frontal gyrus (Broca area) of orchestra musicians.17 The authors of the latter study hypothesized that long-term professional activity to increase visuospatial and audiospatial abilities led to increased gray matter density in the Broca area of orchestra musicians.
Doing mathematics is another unique feature of the brain. Mathematicians have to go through very intense and focused training. After their formal graduate education, mathematicians continue to spend hours, days, and even weeks to concentrate on solving mathematic problems or to create a mathematic model to explain the social and economic events throughout their professional life. In this study, we aimed to investigate whether the long-term intense training and practicing of mathematic thinking could induce structural plasticity in a mathematician's brain. We hypothesized that highly concentrated, focused, and professionally practiced mathematic thinking by mathematicians may create an enriched environment effect to develop experience-dependent structural plastic changes in their brains. To test our hypothesis, we investigated the possible gray and white matter density differences between a group of professional mathematicians (academicians at mathematics departments of universities) and the control subjects.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
We studied 26 mathematicians (19 men and 7 women; mean age, 35.50 ± 6.80 years) who were recruited from the volunteers who have been working as academicians at departments of mathematics. All of the mathematicians completed a 4-year undergraduate program of mathematics and had passed a 4-year-long PhD education program. Four of them were involved in algebra, 14 in applied mathematics, 3 in probabilistic analysis, and 2 in mathematic physics. The native language of all of the mathematicians and control subjects was Turkish. All of the mathematicians and control subjects could speak only English as a foreign language. The mathematicians and control subjects were neither interested in music at a professional level nor could play a musical instrument. One mathematician who was more than 60 years of age and another who could play piano were not included in the study. Exclusion criteria for the mathematicians and control subjects also included any contraindication for MR imaging; alcohol or drug abuse; and any history of neurodegenerative disease, seizure, central nervous system infection, cerebrovascular disease, diabetes mellitus, and head trauma causing loss of consciousness that lasted more than 30 minutes or that required hospitalization. Cerebral lesions other than nonspecific cerebral white matter spots, present on T2-weighted images, were also accepted as exclusion criteria. The mean period of time spent as an academician after completing bachelor's degree was 13.65 ± 6.95 years. The demographic data of the participants are summarized in Table 1.
We recruited 23 sex- and age-matched control subjects (16 men and 7 women) among the volunteers of the academicians from the faculties of medicine and philosophy (mean age, 35.43 ± 7.62 years). All of the mathematicians and control subjects were right-handed (Edinburgh handedness inventory). Informed consent was taken from all of the participants, and the study was approved by the local human subject committee.
Image Acquisition
Cranial MR imaging studies were performed on a 1.5T superconducting whole-body MR imaging system (Symphony Maestro; Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) with a standard quadrature head coil. High-resolution anatomic images of the whole brain were acquired from the mathematicians and control subjects with T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence (TR = 11.08 ms; TE = 4 ms; TI = 300 ms; relaxation delay time = 500 ms; flip angle = 15°; FOV = 256 × 192 mm; matrix size = 256 × 192) yielding 128 sagittal sections with a defined voxel size of 1 × 1 × 1.3 mm. We also obtained the axial fast spin-echo T2-weighted images (TR = 9200 ms; TE = 110 ms; the number of acquisitions = 3) from all of the subjects to search for any pathologic findings defined in the study exclusion criteria.
Voxel-Based Morphometry Protocol and Data Preprocessing
The data preprocessing and analyses were performed with SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm2; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, United Kingdom) running under Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, Mass). MR scanning produces images in Digital Imaging and Communications in Medicine (DICOM) format. All of the images in DICOM format were converted to ANALYSE format with the software MRIcro18 (www.mricro.com). We applied an optimized method of voxel-based morphometry. Preprocessing of the data involved spatial normalization of all of the images into a standardized anatomic space, segmentation into gray and white matter, modulation, and spatial smoothing with a Gaussian kernel.19–21 To reduce the scanner-specific bias, we created a customized gray matter by averaging the smoothened and normalized images of all of the participants. T1-weighted MR images were normalized to the standard T1 template of the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI). The normalized images were smoothed and averaged to obtain a study-specific T1 template. All of the original structural MR images in native space were then normalized to this study-specific template. The normalized images were segmented into CSF, gray matter, and white matter compartments using the SPM2 priors. Afterward, CSF, gray matter, and white matter images were smoothened with an 8-mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) kernel and averaged to obtain study-specific CSF, gray matter, and white matter priors for later segmentation of native images. The original T1-weighted images were segmented with the study-specific T1 template and gray matter, white matter, and CSF priors. This segmentation step involves an affine transformation of each scan to the template with a subsequent back projection into native space. We also performed a correction for volume changes (modulation) by modulating each voxel by the Jacobian determinants derived from the spatial normalization. An automated brain extraction procedure that incorporated a segmentation step was used to remove nonbrain tissue. The extracted gray matter images were then normalized to the group-specific gray matter template. The normalization parameters were then applied to the original structural images in native space to reduce any contribution from nonbrain voxels and afford optimal spatial normalization of gray matter. These normalized images were segmented into gray and white matter. Finally, the normalized and segmented images were smoothed with a 10-mm FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel.
Statistical Analysis
We investigated the regionally specific gray and white matter density differences between the mathematician and control subjects using voxel-by-voxel analysis of variance test. Because the groups did not show a significant difference in age and sex distribution, we did not enter these variables as covariates. For the statistical analysis, we excluded all of the voxels with a gray or white matter value below 0.2 (of a maximum value of 1) to avoid possible edge effects around the border between gray and white matter and to include only voxels with sufficient gray and white matter, respectively. The findings were considered significant at a voxel level of P < .0001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons, with an extended threshold looking for clusters with at least 100 contiguous voxels. The relationship between the time duration of professional work and cortical gray matter density changes was investigated by regression analysis (P < .05, family-wise error [FWE] corrected). For localizing purposes, the MNI coordinates were converted to Talaraich coordinates using a dedicated script (mni2tal.m; available at http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/Imaging/Common/mnispace.shtml). Wherever a significant correlation was found between the mathematicians and control subjects, the gray matter density values were extracted for each of these voxel locations into SPSS for Microsoft Windows (SPSS, Chicago, Ill), and a linear regression analysis was performed to calculate the correlation coefficient. The statistical analyses in this study were carried out by a statistician.
Results
No significant difference was observed in education length and the parental education levels between mathematicians and control subjects (Table 1). In comparison with the age- and sex-matched control subjects, the mathematicians had significantly higher gray matter densities in the right inferior parietal lobule (Talaraich coordinates: x = 54, y = −58, z = 38 [Brodmann area, BA 4]; x = 32, y = −46, z = 60 [BA 7]; x = 57, y = −60, z = 34 [BA 39]); left inferior parietal lobule (x = −38, y = −50, z = 57 [BA 40]); and the left inferior frontal gyrus (x = −42, y = −34, z = 12 [BA 46]; Table 2; Fig 1). There was no region of decreased gray matter density in the mathematicians’ brains compared with control subjects. The regression analyses revealed that the period of time spent as an academician was significantly associated with the gray matter density in the right parietal lobules of the mathematicians (Fig 2A). The gray matter density values extracted from the right inferior parietal lobule (x = 57, y = −60, z = 34 [BA 39]; z = 7.28; P < .05; FEW corrected) showed a strong correlation with the period of time spent as an academician (r = 0.84; P < .01) on the SSPS correlation analysis (Fig 2B). The analysis of images revealed no difference in the white matter density of mathematicians and the control subjects.
Discussion
The relationship between the morphology and functions of the human brain has attracted the interest of neuroscientists since the beginning of 20th century.22,23 Postmortem studies of some famous musicians, intellectual political leaders, and scientists were reported in search of the associations between the outstanding skills and the extraordinary cerebral morphology.24–26 In some of these reports, the famous musicians had larger supramarginal gyrus than the normal population.25 The postmortem study of Albert Einstein's brain showed that his inferior parietal lobule was larger than that of the control subjects and had a very exceptional parietal gyral morphology.26 It has been speculated that there might be an association between the exceptionally enlarged inferior parietal lobules and his outstanding creativity in mathematic thinking. Similarly, the postmortem study of the brain of another famous mathematician, Johann Karl Friedrich Gauss, revealed a remarkably convoluted appearance of the parietal lobes.22,24 Today, we know from the recent in vivo morphometric MR studies that the morphology of the human brain is determined by genetic factors but may also be modified by experiences.
Several cross-sectional morphometry studies have been conducted in musicians to investigate the morphologic differences of their brains from normal control subjects and to search the association between morphologic differences and the training and practicing of musical skills.9–14,16 Results of these studies revealed that musicians had anatomic differences in several regions of their brain that have been involved in motor and auditory processing. The morphologic differences observed in musicians’ brains were significantly correlated with the musical skills and ages of commencement for musical training.13,14 Amunts et al13 reported a strong negative correlation between the commencement age of musical training and sizes of hand-motor cortical regions of pianists. In another study, musicians who began the musical training earlier than age 7 years had larger anterior corpus callosums than the musicians who started the training later.11 Musicians with an absolute pitch skill (ability to discriminate musical tones without a need for a reference tone) had larger planum temporale than the musicians without an absolute pitch.14 Because they are strongly associated with training periods and professional skills, it has been proposed that morphologic differences observed in the brains of musicians were experience-dependent structural changes developed as a result of intense, long-term training and practicing musical skills.9 In the study conducted with the taxi drivers in London, Maguire et al1 reported a significant association between the increased gray matter density in the right posterior hippocampus of the taxi drivers and the period of time spent as a taxi driver. They proposed that long-term training and practicing to memorize a huge amount of spatial information might cause an experience-dependent structural change in the hippocampus of the taxi drivers in which the spatial information is processed. They also found that the control subjects had larger gray matter volumes in the anterior hippocampus. They hypothesized that extensive navigation experience led to redistribution of gray matter in the hippocampus. In our study, we did not observe any finding that might indicate the presence of gray matter redistribution in the mathematicians. Recent longitudinal morphometry studies demonstrated that repeated practicing of a visuomotor task and intense learning of abstract information could lead to gray matter density changes in task-related regions of brain.3,7,27 In summary, the results of cross-sectional and longitudinal in vivo morphometric MR imaging studies point to the presence of experience-dependent structural plasticity in the human brain.
Voxel-based morphometry is a statistical imaging method that gives opportunities to investigate regionally specific subtle gray matter differences between various study groups.19 To the best of our knowledge, this is the first in vivo study investigating morphologic differences of mathematicians’ brains. We hypothesized that long-term and specific training of mathematics and practicing of mathematic thinking might cause an experience-dependent structural alteration in mathematicians’ brains. We conducted an optimized voxel-based morphometry study to test our hypothesis.20,21 Our results suggest that gray matter density of mathematicians shows diverse distribution in the inferior parietal lobules and left inferior frontal gyrus compared with age- and sex-matched control subjects. In addition, we found a strong correlation between the gray matter density in the right inferior parietal lobule of the mathematicians and the period of time spent as an academician. We know from previous functional MR imaging studies that processing of mental arithmetic calculations has language- and visuospatial-related components, which take place in different regions of the brain.28–30 The exact arithmetic calculation skills, such as multiplying, have language-specific representations in the brain and are processed in the language-related left inferior frontal and left angular gyri. The arithmetic calculations requiring approximations and comparison of numbers, such as subtraction, have visuospatial representations in the brain and are processed in the bilateral parietal regions around the intraparietal sulci. In our study, we found significant gray matter density increase in the same regions where the mental arithmetic calculations are processed. The strong correlation between gray matter density change in the right inferior parietal lobule and the period of time spent as an academician indicates that structural difference observed in this region may result from an experience-dependent process. Long-term and intense mental practice with numeric data may lead to experience-dependent plastic changes in the related regions of brain. Mathematicians not only do arithmetic calculations, but they also deal with advanced mathematic problems that require mental imagery, mental creativity, and manipulation of 3D objects, which are all processed in parietal lobes. Mathematicians have a unique ways of scientific thinking, which they use in problem solving. It was stated in a self-description by Albert Einstein as, “… Words and language whether written or spoken, do not seem to play any part in my thought process. The psychological entities that serve as building blocks from my thought are certain signs or images, more or less clear, that I can reproduce or recombine at will.”31 In his statements, he emphasized the visuospatial rather than verbal type of scientific thinking. Another famous mathematician, Jacques Hadamard, declared that his scientific thinking mainly relied on visuospatial rather than verbal processes. Throughout their academic lives, mathematicians practice this visuospatial data processing, which may contribute to the increased gray matter density in the parietal lobules.26
The gray matter changes observed in the left inferior parietal lobule and left inferior frontal gyrus did not correlate with the period of time spent as a mathematician. However, this does not rule out the possibility that gray matter density differences, which are observed in the left inferior frontal gyrus and left inferior parietal lobule, are experience-dependent structural changes. Results of recently reported longitudinal studies revealed that experience-dependent structural changes may develop within weeks or months after the stimuli.7,27 Structural changes observed in left inferior frontal gyrus and left inferior parietal lobule may develop in earlier times of the professional careers of mathematicians. Left inferior frontal gyrus is a language-related region of arithmetic processing. It would be good to test performances on verbal-related arithmetic calculations and to investigate the association between the gray matter density and test performances. Because most of the participants were not willing receive such a test, we were not able to investigate this association.
Histologic correlation of experience-dependent adaptive plasticity in the human brain has not been exactly defined yet. Neurogenesis, microglia proliferation, synaptogenesis, angiogenesis, axonal sprouting, modulation of synaptic transmission, and change in excitatory/inhibitory transmission balance have been postulated as the possible mechanisms of experience-dependent plasticity.32–45 Adaptive neurogenesis in the adult brain was first reported in animal studies. Food hiding and seeking behavior in food-storing bird species, which requires the storage of spatial information, stimulates the neurogenesis in the hippocampal region known as the spatial navigation center.38,44 Male canaries sing during their breeding season and learn new songs every year. Just before their breeding season, the high vocal center (a region in the brains of birds that is related to singing) of male canaries enlarges with the addition of new neurons.38,45 It has also been demonstrated that enriched environments in the experimental conditions may stimulate the neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult mammalian brains.43 Synaptogenesis is the other mechanism that has been postulated to explain the mechanism of experience-dependent plasticity.32,42,43,46 Long-term sensory stimuli increase the number of synapses that modify the sensitivity of sensory system. Thus, newly generated synapses increase the complexity of the neuronal network that may enable the processing of complex data.
It is known from the previous studies that intelligence quotient (IQ) levels of individuals may influence the cortical gray matter distribution. It would be good to perform IQ measurements and consider IQ scores as a cofactor in the statistical analyses (analysis of covariance) of voxel-based morphometry. However, most of our subjects did not agree to get an IQ test. However, there was no significant difference in the education levels between the study groups, and all of the subjects in both groups were academicians. So, the individuals from both groups would be expected to have similar IQ levels. There is no objective evidence for a significant IQ level difference between the mathematicians and control subjects.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we found that mathematicians had significantly increased cortical gray matter density in the bilateral inferior parietal lobules and left inferior frontal gyrus, which are known to be involved in arithmetic calculations and visuospatial processing. Significant association of gray matter density changes in the right inferior parietal lobule with the period of time spent as an academician proposed that the structural change in this location is an experience-dependent process, which might be caused by the long-term practices of visuospatial and arithmetic data processing.
References
- Received January 25, 2007.
- Accepted after revision April 11, 2007.
- Copyright © American Society of Neuroradiology